Example of Psychophysiology format
Recent searches

Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format
Sample paper formatted on SciSpace - SciSpace
This content is only for preview purposes. The original open access content can be found here.
Look Inside
Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format Example of Psychophysiology format
Sample paper formatted on SciSpace - SciSpace
This content is only for preview purposes. The original open access content can be found here.
open access Open Access
recommended Recommended

Psychophysiology — Template for authors

Publisher: Wiley
Categories Rank Trend in last 3 yrs
Experimental and Cognitive Psychology #14 of 148 up up by 6 ranks
Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology #6 of 60 up up by 7 ranks
Cognitive Neuroscience #21 of 96 down down by None rank
Developmental Neuroscience #8 of 35 down down by None rank
Neurology #34 of 156 down down by None rank
Endocrine and Autonomic Systems #7 of 21 down down by None rank
Neuroscience (all) #36 of 110 down down by None rank
Biological Psychiatry #15 of 38 down down by None rank
journal-quality-icon Journal quality:
High
calendar-icon Last 4 years overview: 678 Published Papers | 4315 Citations
indexed-in-icon Indexed in: Scopus
last-updated-icon Last updated: 13/07/2020
Related journals
Insights
General info
Top papers
Popular templates
Get started guide
Why choose from SciSpace
FAQ

Related Journals

open access Open Access

Frontiers Media

Quality:  
High
CiteRatio: 5.1
SJR: 1.128
SNIP: 1.221
open access Open Access
recommended Recommended

Elsevier

Quality:  
High
CiteRatio: 25.5
SJR: 6.857
SNIP: 5.733
open access Open Access

Elsevier

Quality:  
High
CiteRatio: 4.8
SJR: 1.245
SNIP: 1.132
open access Open Access

SAGE

Quality:  
High
CiteRatio: 4.3
SJR: 1.395
SNIP: 2.063

Journal Performance & Insights

Impact Factor

CiteRatio

Determines the importance of a journal by taking a measure of frequency with which the average article in a journal has been cited in a particular year.

A measure of average citations received per peer-reviewed paper published in the journal.

3.692

9% from 2018

Impact factor for Psychophysiology from 2016 - 2019
Year Value
2019 3.692
2018 3.378
2017 3.118
2016 2.668
graph view Graph view
table view Table view

6.4

2% from 2019

CiteRatio for Psychophysiology from 2016 - 2020
Year Value
2020 6.4
2019 6.5
2018 6.0
2017 5.2
2016 5.3
graph view Graph view
table view Table view

insights Insights

  • Impact factor of this journal has increased by 9% in last year.
  • This journal’s impact factor is in the top 10 percentile category.

insights Insights

  • CiteRatio of this journal has decreased by 2% in last years.
  • This journal’s CiteRatio is in the top 10 percentile category.

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR)

Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP)

Measures weighted citations received by the journal. Citation weighting depends on the categories and prestige of the citing journal.

Measures actual citations received relative to citations expected for the journal's category.

1.661

8% from 2019

SJR for Psychophysiology from 2016 - 2020
Year Value
2020 1.661
2019 1.809
2018 1.777
2017 1.616
2016 1.618
graph view Graph view
table view Table view

1.567

2% from 2019

SNIP for Psychophysiology from 2016 - 2020
Year Value
2020 1.567
2019 1.533
2018 1.375
2017 1.237
2016 1.042
graph view Graph view
table view Table view

insights Insights

  • SJR of this journal has decreased by 8% in last years.
  • This journal’s SJR is in the top 10 percentile category.

insights Insights

  • SNIP of this journal has increased by 2% in last years.
  • This journal’s SNIP is in the top 10 percentile category.

Psychophysiology

Guideline source: View

All company, product and service names used in this website are for identification purposes only. All product names, trademarks and registered trademarks are property of their respective owners.

Use of these names, trademarks and brands does not imply endorsement or affiliation. Disclaimer Notice

Wiley

Psychophysiology

Psychophysiology is the oldest, first, and most established journal in its field. This prestigious international journal plays a key role in advancing psychophysiological science and human neuroscience, covering research on the interrelationships between the physiological and ...... Read More

Psychology

i
Last updated on
13 Jul 2020
i
ISSN
0048-5772
i
Impact Factor
High - 1.222
i
Open Access
Yes
i
Sherpa RoMEO Archiving Policy
Yellow faq
i
Plagiarism Check
Available via Turnitin
i
Endnote Style
Download Available
i
Bibliography Name
apa
i
Citation Type
Numbered
[25]
i
Bibliography Example
Beenakker, C.W.J. (2006) Specular andreev reflection in graphene.Phys. Rev. Lett., 97 (6), 067 007. URL 10.1103/PhysRevLett.97.067007.

Top papers written in this journal

open accessOpen access Journal Article DOI: 10.1017/S0048577201393198
Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences
James J. Gross1
01 May 2002 - Psychophysiology

Abstract:

One of life's great challenges is successfully regulating emotions. Do some emotion regulation strategies have more to recommend them than others? According to Gross's (1998, Review of General Psychology, 2, 271-299) process model of emotion regulation, strategies that act early in the emotion-generative process should have a... One of life's great challenges is successfully regulating emotions. Do some emotion regulation strategies have more to recommend them than others? According to Gross's (1998, Review of General Psychology, 2, 271-299) process model of emotion regulation, strategies that act early in the emotion-generative process should have a different profile of consequences than strategies that act later on. This review focuses on two commonly used strategies for down-regulating emotion. The first, reappraisal, comes early in the emotion-generative process. It consists of changing the way a situation is construed so as to decrease its emotional impact. The second, suppression, comes later in the emotion-generative process. It consists of inhibiting the outward signs of inner feelings. Experimental and individual-difference studies find reappraisal is often more effective than suppression. Reappraisal decreases emotion experience and behavioral expression, and has no impact on memory. By contrast, suppression decreases behavioral expression, but fails to decrease emotion experience, and actually impairs memory. Suppression also increases physiological responding for suppressors and their social partners. This review concludes with a consideration of five important directions for future research on emotion regulation processes. read more read less

Topics:

Expressive Suppression (67%)67% related to the paper, Cognitive reappraisal (64%)64% related to the paper, Affective science (62%)62% related to the paper, Two-factor theory of emotion (62%)62% related to the paper, Emotion work (61%)61% related to the paper
View PDF
3,555 Citations
open accessOpen access Journal Article DOI: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.1997.TB02140.X
Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats
01 Nov 1997 - Psychophysiology

Abstract:

Components of heart rate variability have attracted considerable attention in psychology and medicine and have become important dependent measures in psychophysiology and behavioral medicine. Quantification and interpretation of heart rate variability, however, remain complex issues and are fraught with pitfalls. The present ... Components of heart rate variability have attracted considerable attention in psychology and medicine and have become important dependent measures in psychophysiology and behavioral medicine. Quantification and interpretation of heart rate variability, however, remain complex issues and are fraught with pitfalls. The present report (a) examines the physiological origins and mechanisms of heart rate variability, (b) considers quantitative approaches to measurement, and (c) highlights important caveats in the interpretation of heart rate variability. Summary guidelines for research in this area are outlined, and suggestions and prospects for future developments are considered. read more read less

Topics:

Heart rate variability (51%)51% related to the paper
View PDF
3,273 Citations
Journal Article DOI: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.1987.TB00311.X
The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure
Risto Näätänen1, Terence W. Picton1
01 Jul 1987 - Psychophysiology

Abstract:

This paper reviews the literature on the Nl wave of the human auditory evoked potential. It concludes that at least six different cerebral processes can contribute to (he negative wave recorded from the scalp with a peak latency between 50 and 150 ms: a component generated in the auditory-cortex on the supratemporal plane, a ... This paper reviews the literature on the Nl wave of the human auditory evoked potential. It concludes that at least six different cerebral processes can contribute to (he negative wave recorded from the scalp with a peak latency between 50 and 150 ms: a component generated in the auditory-cortex on the supratemporal plane, a component generated in the association cortex on the lateral aspect of the temporal and parietal cortex, a component generated in the motor and premotor cortices, the mismatch negativity, a temporal component of the processing negativity, and a frontal component of the processing negativity, The first three, which can be considered ‘true’ N1 components, are controlled by the physical and temporal aspects of the stimulus and by the general state of the subject. The other three components are not necessarily elicited by a stimulus but depend on the conditions in which the stimulus occurs. They often last much longer than the true N1 components that they overlap. read more read less

Topics:

Mismatch negativity (58%)58% related to the paper, N100 (57%)57% related to the paper, Auditory evoked field (53%)53% related to the paper, P3a (52%)52% related to the paper, Contingent negative variation (52%)52% related to the paper
3,137 Citations
Journal Article DOI: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.1993.TB03352.X
Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions
Peter Lang1, Mark K. Greenwald1, Margaret M. Bradley1, Alfons O. Hamm1
01 May 1993 - Psychophysiology

Abstract:

Colored photographic pictures that varied widely across the affective dimensions of valence (pleasant-unpleasant) and arousal (excited-calm) were each viewed for a 6-s period while facial electromyographic (zygomatic and corrugator muscle activity) and visceral (heart rate and skin conductance) reactions were measured. Judgme... Colored photographic pictures that varied widely across the affective dimensions of valence (pleasant-unpleasant) and arousal (excited-calm) were each viewed for a 6-s period while facial electromyographic (zygomatic and corrugator muscle activity) and visceral (heart rate and skin conductance) reactions were measured. Judgments relating to pleasure, arousal, interest, and emotional state were measured, as was choice viewing time. Significant covariation was obtained between (a) facial expression and affective valence judgments and (b) skin conductance magnitude and arousal ratings. Interest ratings and viewing time were also associated with arousal. Although differences due to the subject's gender and cognitive style were obtained, affective responses were largely independent of the personality factors investigated. Response specificity, particularly facial expressiveness, supported the view that specific affects have unique patterns of reactivity. The consistency of the dimensional relationships between evaluative judgments (i.e., pleasure and arousal) and physiological response, however, emphasizes that emotion is fundamentally organized by these motivational parameters. read more read less

Topics:

International Affective Picture System (61%)61% related to the paper, Facial electromyography (56%)56% related to the paper, Facial expression (54%)54% related to the paper, Valence (psychology) (53%)53% related to the paper, Arousal (53%)53% related to the paper
3,089 Citations
Journal Article DOI: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720163
Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation.
01 Mar 2000 - Psychophysiology

Abstract:

Eye movements, eye blinks, cardiac signals, muscle noise, and line noise present serious problems for electroencephalographic (EEG) interpretation and analysis when rejecting contaminated EEG segments results in an unacceptable data loss. Many methods have been proposed to remove artifacts from EEG recordings, especially thos... Eye movements, eye blinks, cardiac signals, muscle noise, and line noise present serious problems for electroencephalographic (EEG) interpretation and analysis when rejecting contaminated EEG segments results in an unacceptable data loss. Many methods have been proposed to remove artifacts from EEG recordings, especially those arising from eye movements and blinks. Often regression in the time or frequency domain is performed on parallel EEG and electrooculographic (EOG) recordings to derive parameters characterizing the appearance and spread of EOG artifacts in the EEG channels. Because EEG and ocular activity mix bidirectionally, regressing out eye artifacts inevitably involves subtracting relevant EEG signals from each record as well. Regression methods become even more problematic when a good regressing channel is not available for each artifact source, as in the case of muscle artifacts. Use of principal component analysis (PCA) has been proposed to remove eye artifacts from multichannel EEG. However, PCA cannot completely separate eye artifacts from brain signals, especially when they have comparable amplitudes. Here, we propose a new and generally applicable method for removing a wide variety of artifacts from EEG records based on blind source separation by independent component analysis (ICA). Our results on EEG data collected from normal and autistic subjects show that ICA can effectively detect, separate, and remove contamination from a wide variety of artifactual sources in EEG records with results comparing favorably with those obtained using regression and PCA methods. ICA can also be used to analyze blink-related brain activity. read more read less

Topics:

Artifact (error) (57%)57% related to the paper, Blind signal separation (51%)51% related to the paper, Electroencephalography (51%)51% related to the paper
2,944 Citations
Author Pic

SciSpace is a very innovative solution to the formatting problem and existing providers, such as Mendeley or Word did not really evolve in recent years.

- Andreas Frutiger, Researcher, ETH Zurich, Institute for Biomedical Engineering

Get MS-Word and LaTeX output to any Journal within seconds
1
Choose a template
Select a template from a library of 40,000+ templates
2
Import a MS-Word file or start fresh
It takes only few seconds to import
3
View and edit your final output
SciSpace will automatically format your output to meet journal guidelines
4
Submit directly or Download
Submit to journal directly or Download in PDF, MS Word or LaTeX

(Before submission check for plagiarism via Turnitin)

clock Less than 3 minutes

What to expect from SciSpace?

Speed and accuracy over MS Word

''

With SciSpace, you do not need a word template for Psychophysiology.

It automatically formats your research paper to Wiley formatting guidelines and citation style.

You can download a submission ready research paper in pdf, LaTeX and docx formats.

Time comparison

Time taken to format a paper and Compliance with guidelines

Plagiarism Reports via Turnitin

SciSpace has partnered with Turnitin, the leading provider of Plagiarism Check software.

Using this service, researchers can compare submissions against more than 170 million scholarly articles, a database of 70+ billion current and archived web pages. How Turnitin Integration works?

Turnitin Stats
Publisher Logos

Freedom from formatting guidelines

One editor, 100K journal formats – world's largest collection of journal templates

With such a huge verified library, what you need is already there.

publisher-logos

Easy support from all your favorite tools

Psychophysiology format uses apa citation style.

Automatically format and order your citations and bibliography in a click.

SciSpace allows imports from all reference managers like Mendeley, Zotero, Endnote, Google Scholar etc.

Frequently asked questions

1. Can I write Psychophysiology in LaTeX?

Absolutely not! Our tool has been designed to help you focus on writing. You can write your entire paper as per the Psychophysiology guidelines and auto format it.

2. Do you follow the Psychophysiology guidelines?

Yes, the template is compliant with the Psychophysiology guidelines. Our experts at SciSpace ensure that. If there are any changes to the journal's guidelines, we'll change our algorithm accordingly.

3. Can I cite my article in multiple styles in Psychophysiology?

Of course! We support all the top citation styles, such as APA style, MLA style, Vancouver style, Harvard style, and Chicago style. For example, when you write your paper and hit autoformat, our system will automatically update your article as per the Psychophysiology citation style.

4. Can I use the Psychophysiology templates for free?

Sign up for our free trial, and you'll be able to use all our features for seven days. You'll see how helpful they are and how inexpensive they are compared to other options, Especially for Psychophysiology.

5. Can I use a manuscript in Psychophysiology that I have written in MS Word?

Yes. You can choose the right template, copy-paste the contents from the word document, and click on auto-format. Once you're done, you'll have a publish-ready paper Psychophysiology that you can download at the end.

6. How long does it usually take you to format my papers in Psychophysiology?

It only takes a matter of seconds to edit your manuscript. Besides that, our intuitive editor saves you from writing and formatting it in Psychophysiology.

7. Where can I find the template for the Psychophysiology?

It is possible to find the Word template for any journal on Google. However, why use a template when you can write your entire manuscript on SciSpace , auto format it as per Psychophysiology's guidelines and download the same in Word, PDF and LaTeX formats? Give us a try!.

8. Can I reformat my paper to fit the Psychophysiology's guidelines?

Of course! You can do this using our intuitive editor. It's very easy. If you need help, our support team is always ready to assist you.

9. Psychophysiology an online tool or is there a desktop version?

SciSpace's Psychophysiology is currently available as an online tool. We're developing a desktop version, too. You can request (or upvote) any features that you think would be helpful for you and other researchers in the "feature request" section of your account once you've signed up with us.

10. I cannot find my template in your gallery. Can you create it for me like Psychophysiology?

Sure. You can request any template and we'll have it setup within a few days. You can find the request box in Journal Gallery on the right side bar under the heading, "Couldn't find the format you were looking for like Psychophysiology?”

11. What is the output that I would get after using Psychophysiology?

After writing your paper autoformatting in Psychophysiology, you can download it in multiple formats, viz., PDF, Docx, and LaTeX.

12. Is Psychophysiology's impact factor high enough that I should try publishing my article there?

To be honest, the answer is no. The impact factor is one of the many elements that determine the quality of a journal. Few of these factors include review board, rejection rates, frequency of inclusion in indexes, and Eigenfactor. You need to assess all these factors before you make your final call.

13. What is Sherpa RoMEO Archiving Policy for Psychophysiology?

SHERPA/RoMEO Database

We extracted this data from Sherpa Romeo to help researchers understand the access level of this journal in accordance with the Sherpa Romeo Archiving Policy for Psychophysiology. The table below indicates the level of access a journal has as per Sherpa Romeo's archiving policy.

RoMEO Colour Archiving policy
Green Can archive pre-print and post-print or publisher's version/PDF
Blue Can archive post-print (ie final draft post-refereeing) or publisher's version/PDF
Yellow Can archive pre-print (ie pre-refereeing)
White Archiving not formally supported
FYI:
  1. Pre-prints as being the version of the paper before peer review and
  2. Post-prints as being the version of the paper after peer-review, with revisions having been made.

14. What are the most common citation types In Psychophysiology?

The 5 most common citation types in order of usage for Psychophysiology are:.

S. No. Citation Style Type
1. Author Year
2. Numbered
3. Numbered (Superscripted)
4. Author Year (Cited Pages)
5. Footnote

15. How do I submit my article to the Psychophysiology?

It is possible to find the Word template for any journal on Google. However, why use a template when you can write your entire manuscript on SciSpace , auto format it as per Psychophysiology's guidelines and download the same in Word, PDF and LaTeX formats? Give us a try!.

16. Can I download Psychophysiology in Endnote format?

Yes, SciSpace provides this functionality. After signing up, you would need to import your existing references from Word or Bib file to SciSpace. Then SciSpace would allow you to download your references in Psychophysiology Endnote style according to Elsevier guidelines.

Fast and reliable,
built for complaince.

Instant formatting to 100% publisher guidelines on - SciSpace.

Available only on desktops 🖥

No word template required

Typset automatically formats your research paper to Psychophysiology formatting guidelines and citation style.

Verifed journal formats

One editor, 100K journal formats.
With the largest collection of verified journal formats, what you need is already there.

Trusted by academicians

I spent hours with MS word for reformatting. It was frustrating - plain and simple. With SciSpace, I can draft my manuscripts and once it is finished I can just submit. In case, I have to submit to another journal it is really just a button click instead of an afternoon of reformatting.

Andreas Frutiger
Researcher & Ex MS Word user
Use this template